|
|
xiii | |
Preface |
|
xv | |
Dedication |
|
xvii | |
Commentary |
|
xxiii | |
|
|
1 Byron A. Campbell: Selected Works |
|
|
|
Retention and Extinction of Learned Fear in Infant and Adult Rats (1962) |
|
|
1 | (15) |
|
|
|
|
16 | (5) |
|
|
|
2 Age-Related Changes in Imitation: Implications for Memory Development |
|
|
21 | (48) |
|
|
|
Mobile Conjugate Reinforcement Paradigm |
|
|
21 | (3) |
|
|
24 | (2) |
|
|
24 | (1) |
|
|
25 | (1) |
|
|
26 | (31) |
|
|
27 | (1) |
|
Age-Related Changes in Learning |
|
|
28 | (1) |
|
|
28 | (4) |
|
Imitation From Television |
|
|
32 | (6) |
|
Age-Related Changes in Retention |
|
|
38 | (2) |
|
Age-Related Changes in the Specificity of Memory Retrieval |
|
|
40 | (2) |
|
|
42 | (5) |
|
Contextual Retrieval Cues |
|
|
47 | (2) |
|
|
49 | (1) |
|
Long-Term Retention of Complex Sequences |
|
|
50 | (1) |
|
Specificity of Effective Retrieval Cues |
|
|
51 | (3) |
|
|
54 | (3) |
|
|
57 | (2) |
|
Implications and Conclusions |
|
|
59 | (3) |
|
|
59 | (2) |
|
|
61 | (1) |
|
|
62 | (1) |
|
|
62 | (7) |
|
3 Down Syndrome: Neuropsychology and Animal Models |
|
|
69 | (44) |
|
|
|
The Neuropsychology Of Down Syndrome |
|
|
69 | (9) |
|
Level and Trajectory of IQ |
|
|
72 | (1) |
|
|
73 | (1) |
|
|
74 | (1) |
|
|
74 | (1) |
|
|
75 | (2) |
|
|
77 | (1) |
|
|
77 | (1) |
|
Overview Of Animal Models Of Down Syndrome |
|
|
78 | (6) |
|
Why Create Animal Models of DS? |
|
|
78 | (1) |
|
|
79 | (1) |
|
Development of Animal Models of Down Syndrome |
|
|
79 | (1) |
|
|
80 | (1) |
|
Single Gene and Yeast Artificial Chromosome (YAC) Transgenics |
|
|
81 | (2) |
|
|
83 | (1) |
|
Developmental and Behavioral Phenotype Of Segmental Trisomies |
|
|
84 | (18) |
|
|
84 | (1) |
|
|
84 | (1) |
|
Cognitive Phenotype of the Partial Trisomies |
|
|
85 | (1) |
|
|
86 | (4) |
|
|
90 | (6) |
|
|
96 | (1) |
|
|
96 | (1) |
|
|
96 | (1) |
|
|
97 | (1) |
|
Central Nervous System Phenotype of Segmental Trisomies |
|
|
97 | (1) |
|
|
97 | (1) |
|
|
98 | (1) |
|
Alzheimer's-Like Neuropathology |
|
|
99 | (1) |
|
|
99 | (1) |
|
Gene Linkage of Cognitive Phenotypes |
|
|
100 | (1) |
|
|
101 | (1) |
|
|
102 | (1) |
|
|
102 | (1) |
|
|
103 | (10) |
|
4 The Development of Visual Surface Perception: Insights Into the Ontogeny of Knowledge |
|
|
113 | (42) |
|
|
The Infant's Visual World |
|
|
116 | (7) |
|
Young Infants' Perception of Object Unity |
|
|
116 | (3) |
|
Visual Information in the Three-Dimensional Rod-and-Box Display |
|
|
119 | (1) |
|
Three-Dimensional Depth Cues |
|
|
119 | (1) |
|
Accretion and Deletion of Texture |
|
|
120 | (1) |
|
|
120 | (1) |
|
Interposition of Rod-and-Box Edges |
|
|
120 | (1) |
|
Configurational Information |
|
|
120 | (1) |
|
Surface Appearance (Texture, Luminance, and Color) |
|
|
121 | (1) |
|
Models of Young Infants' Perception of Object Unity |
|
|
121 | (2) |
|
Testing Individual Cues In Young Infants' Surface Perception |
|
|
123 | (14) |
|
|
123 | (1) |
|
Three-Dimensional Depth Cues |
|
|
123 | (1) |
|
Accretion and Deletion of Background Texture |
|
|
124 | (1) |
|
Edge Sensitivity: The Case of Illusory Contours |
|
|
125 | (3) |
|
|
128 | (1) |
|
|
128 | (1) |
|
|
128 | (2) |
|
|
130 | (1) |
|
Configuration of the Occluder: Integration of Visual Information Over Space and Time |
|
|
130 | (1) |
|
Configuration of the Partly Occluded Object: The Role of Good Form |
|
|
131 | (1) |
|
Locally Misaligned Object Edges |
|
|
132 | (1) |
|
Locally Nonaligned Object Edges |
|
|
133 | (1) |
|
Common Fate (Synchronous Change) |
|
|
133 | (1) |
|
|
134 | (2) |
|
|
136 | (1) |
|
A Theoretical Description of the Results: The Threshold Model |
|
|
136 | (1) |
|
The Development Of Surface Perception: Empirical Findings |
|
|
137 | (2) |
|
Evidence for Innate Knowledge? |
|
|
137 | (1) |
|
The Development of Perception of Object Unity: Evidence From Infants |
|
|
137 | (2) |
|
The Development Of Surface Perception: Theoretical Considerations And Hypotheses |
|
|
139 | (8) |
|
The Development of Perception of Object Unity: A Computational Model |
|
|
140 | (1) |
|
The Neurophysiology of Vision |
|
|
141 | (1) |
|
The Neurophysiology of Object Perception |
|
|
142 | (1) |
|
A Threshold for the Development of Surface Perception? |
|
|
143 | (1) |
|
Development of the Visual Cortex and Surface Perception |
|
|
144 | (2) |
|
Is Object Knowledge Innate? |
|
|
146 | (1) |
|
|
147 | (1) |
|
|
148 | (1) |
|
|
149 | (1) |
|
|
149 | (6) |
|
5 Automated Face Analysis |
|
|
155 | (28) |
|
|
|
|
|
|
Current Methods Of Analyzing Facial Displays |
|
|
156 | (3) |
|
The Need For Automated Methods Of Analyzing Facial Displays |
|
|
159 | (1) |
|
Current State Of Automated Methods |
|
|
159 | (6) |
|
|
159 | (1) |
|
|
160 | (1) |
|
Principal Components Analysis of Gray-Scale Images |
|
|
160 | (2) |
|
Tracking Standard Facial Features |
|
|
162 | (1) |
|
|
162 | (3) |
|
High-Gradient Component Detection |
|
|
165 | (1) |
|
|
165 | (9) |
|
|
166 | (1) |
|
|
166 | (1) |
|
High-Gradient Component Detection |
|
|
167 | (2) |
|
|
169 | (2) |
|
Hidden Markov Modeling of FACS Action Units |
|
|
171 | (3) |
|
|
174 | (1) |
|
|
174 | (1) |
|
|
174 | (1) |
|
Action Unit Discrimination |
|
|
174 | (1) |
|
|
175 | (2) |
|
|
177 | (1) |
|
|
177 | (6) |
|
6 Texture Segmentation, "Pop-out," and Feature Binding in Infants and Children |
|
|
183 | (68) |
|
|
|
185 | (3) |
|
Texture Segmentation And Pop-Out: Investigations With The Forced-Choice Preferential Looking Method |
|
|
188 | (14) |
|
Segmentation of Oriented and Luminance-Defined Textures Presented on Two Separate Test Fields (Experiment 1) |
|
|
188 | (1) |
|
|
189 | (2) |
|
|
191 | (3) |
|
|
194 | (1) |
|
Infants' Segmentation of Oriented Textures Presented on a Single, Continuous Test Field (Experiment 2) |
|
|
195 | (1) |
|
|
196 | (1) |
|
|
197 | (3) |
|
|
200 | (2) |
|
Texture Segmentation And Pop-Out: Assessments With A Habituation-Novelty Preference Procedure |
|
|
202 | (10) |
|
Infants' Segmentation of Oriented Textures Presented on Two Separate Test Fields (Experiment 3) |
|
|
202 | (1) |
|
|
202 | (2) |
|
|
204 | (1) |
|
|
205 | (3) |
|
Do Infants Perceive an Embedded-Oriented Texture as a Coherent Figure? (Experiment 4) |
|
|
208 | (1) |
|
|
208 | (1) |
|
|
209 | (1) |
|
|
210 | (2) |
|
Infants' Perception Of Subjective Contours: A Particular Case Of Feature Binding? (Experiment 5) |
|
|
212 | (10) |
|
Do Infants Prefer Stimuli Containing a Subjective Contour? (Experiment 5.1) |
|
|
213 | (1) |
|
|
213 | (1) |
|
|
214 | (1) |
|
Do Infants Perceive the Orientation of the Subjective Contour? (Experiment 5.2) |
|
|
215 | (1) |
|
|
216 | (1) |
|
|
217 | (1) |
|
Do Infants Perceive the Subjective Contour as a Line of a Particular Orientation? (Experiment 5.3) |
|
|
218 | (1) |
|
|
219 | (1) |
|
|
220 | (1) |
|
|
221 | (1) |
|
Segmentation Of Oriented And Luminance-Defined Textures Presented On Test Cards (Control Experiments) |
|
|
222 | (3) |
|
|
223 | (1) |
|
|
223 | (2) |
|
|
225 | (1) |
|
The Role Of Saliency In Texture Segmentation (Experiment 6) |
|
|
225 | (10) |
|
Do Infants Prefer the Least Salient Texture if Stimuli Are Presented on Two Separate Test Fields? (Experiment 6.1) |
|
|
226 | (1) |
|
|
226 | (1) |
|
|
227 | (1) |
|
Do Infants Prefer the Least Salient Texture if Stimuli Are Presented on Test Cards? (Experiment 6.2) |
|
|
228 | (1) |
|
|
228 | (2) |
|
|
230 | (2) |
|
|
232 | (3) |
|
Texture Segmentation And Pop-Out In School Children: Assessments With A Manual Reaction Time Procedure (Experiment 7) |
|
|
235 | (3) |
|
|
235 | (1) |
|
|
235 | (2) |
|
|
237 | (1) |
|
|
238 | (7) |
|
|
238 | (1) |
|
Relationship to Previous Studies |
|
|
239 | (2) |
|
Do Young Infants Experience Pop-out? |
|
|
241 | (1) |
|
Possible Neural Mechanisms for Segmentation and Binding |
|
|
242 | (2) |
|
Relation to Animal Studies |
|
|
244 | (1) |
|
|
244 | (1) |
|
|
245 | (1) |
|
|
245 | (6) |
|
7 The Continuum of Language Development During Infancy and Early Childhood: Electrophysiological Correlates |
|
|
251 | (38) |
|
|
|
The Use Of Erp Technique To Study Brain-Behavior Relations |
|
|
252 | (2) |
|
Phonetic Discrimination and Erps |
|
|
254 | (15) |
|
|
255 | (5) |
|
|
260 | (4) |
|
|
264 | (5) |
|
Multivariate Approaches To Language Prediction Using Erps To Predict Later Development |
|
|
269 | (7) |
|
|
269 | (2) |
|
Studies With Preschool Children |
|
|
271 | (2) |
|
Studies With School-Age Children |
|
|
273 | (3) |
|
Why Are Erps Predictive Of Language Development? |
|
|
276 | (6) |
|
|
282 | (1) |
|
|
283 | (1) |
|
|
283 | (6) |
Author Index |
|
289 | (12) |
Subject Index |
|
301 | |