Preface to the Third Edition |
|
xxv | |
About the Companion Website |
|
xxviii | |
|
|
1 | (9) |
|
|
1 | (1) |
|
|
2 | (1) |
|
|
3 | (2) |
|
1.4 Other Sources of Knowledge |
|
|
5 | (1) |
|
1.5 Notation and Terminology |
|
|
6 | (3) |
|
1.5.1 Clinical Trial Terminology |
|
|
7 | (1) |
|
1.5.2 Drug Development Traditionally Recognizes Four Trial Design Types |
|
|
7 | (1) |
|
1.5.3 Descriptive Terminology Is Better |
|
|
8 | (1) |
|
1.6 Examples, Data, and Programs |
|
|
9 | (1) |
|
|
9 | (1) |
|
2 Clinical Trials as Research |
|
|
10 | (33) |
|
|
10 | (3) |
|
|
13 | (6) |
|
|
13 | (1) |
|
2.2.2 Clinical Reasoning Is Based on the Case History |
|
|
14 | (2) |
|
2.2.3 Statistical Reasoning Emphasizes Inference Based on Designed Data Production |
|
|
16 | (1) |
|
2.2.4 Clinical and Statistical Reasoning Converge in Research |
|
|
17 | (2) |
|
2.3 Denning Clinical Trials |
|
|
19 | (10) |
|
2.3.1 Mixing of Clinical and Statistical Reasoning Is Recent |
|
|
19 | (2) |
|
2.3.2 Clinical Trials Are Rigorously Denned |
|
|
21 | (1) |
|
|
22 | (1) |
|
2.3.4 Experiments Can Be Misunderstood |
|
|
23 | (2) |
|
2.3.5 Clinical Trials and the Frankenstein Myth |
|
|
25 | (1) |
|
|
26 | (1) |
|
2.3.7 Clinical Trials as Science |
|
|
26 | (2) |
|
2.3.8 Trials and Statistical Methods Fit within a Spectrum of Clinical Research |
|
|
28 | (1) |
|
2.4 Practicalities of Usage |
|
|
29 | (6) |
|
2.4.1 Predicates for a Trial |
|
|
29 | (1) |
|
2.4.2 Trials Can Provide Confirmatory Evidence |
|
|
29 | (1) |
|
2.4.3 Clinical Trials Are Reliable Albeit Unwieldy and Messy |
|
|
30 | (1) |
|
2.4.4 Trials Are Difficult to Apply in Some Circumstances |
|
|
31 | (1) |
|
2.4.5 Randomized Studies Can Be Initiated Early |
|
|
32 | (1) |
|
2.4.6 What Can I learn from n = 20? |
|
|
33 | (2) |
|
2.5 Nonexperimental Designs |
|
|
35 | (6) |
|
2.5.1 Other Methods Are Valid for Making Some Clinical Inferences |
|
|
35 | (3) |
|
2.5.2 Some Specific Nonexperimental Designs |
|
|
38 | (2) |
|
2.5.3 Causal Relationships |
|
|
40 | (1) |
|
2.5.4 Will Genetic Determinism Replace Design? |
|
|
41 | (1) |
|
|
41 | (1) |
|
2.7 Questions for Discussion |
|
|
41 | (2) |
|
3 Why Clinical Trials Are Ethical |
|
|
43 | (44) |
|
|
43 | (4) |
|
3.1.1 Science and Ethics Share Objectives |
|
|
44 | (2) |
|
3.1.2 Equipoise and Uncertainty |
|
|
46 | (1) |
|
|
47 | (10) |
|
3.2.1 Clinical Trials Sharpen, But Do Not Create, Duality |
|
|
47 | (1) |
|
3.2.2 A Gene Therapy Tragedy Illustrates Duality |
|
|
48 | (1) |
|
3.2.3 Research and Practice Are Convergent |
|
|
48 | (4) |
|
3.2.4 Hippocratic Tradition Does Not Proscribe Clinical Trials |
|
|
52 | (2) |
|
3.2.5 Physicians Always Have Multiple Roles |
|
|
54 | (3) |
|
3.3 Historically Derived Principles of Ethics |
|
|
57 | (8) |
|
3.3.1 Nuremberg Contributed an Awareness of the Worst Problems |
|
|
57 | (1) |
|
3.3.2 High-Profile Mistakes Were Made in the United States |
|
|
58 | (1) |
|
3.3.3 The Helsinki Declaration Was Widely Adopted |
|
|
58 | (3) |
|
3.3.4 Other International Guidelines Have Been Proposed |
|
|
61 | (1) |
|
3.3.5 Institutional Review Boards Provide Ethics Oversight |
|
|
62 | (1) |
|
3.3.6 Ethics Principles Relevant to Clinical Trials |
|
|
63 | (2) |
|
3.4 Contemporary Foundational Principles |
|
|
65 | (7) |
|
3.4.1 Collaborative Partnership |
|
|
66 | (1) |
|
|
66 | (1) |
|
3.4.3 Scientific Validity |
|
|
66 | (1) |
|
3.4.4 Fair Subject Selection |
|
|
67 | (1) |
|
3.4.5 Favorable Risk-Benefit |
|
|
67 | (1) |
|
|
68 | (1) |
|
|
68 | (3) |
|
3.4.8 Respect for Subjects |
|
|
71 | (1) |
|
3.5 Methodologic Reflections |
|
|
72 | (7) |
|
3.5.1 Practice Based on Unproven Treatments Is Not Ethical |
|
|
72 | (2) |
|
3.5.2 Ethics Considerations Are Important Determinants of Design |
|
|
74 | (1) |
|
3.5.3 Specific Methods Have Justification |
|
|
75 | (4) |
|
|
79 | (6) |
|
|
79 | (2) |
|
3.6.2 Physician to Physician Communication Is Not Research |
|
|
81 | (1) |
|
3.6.3 Investigator Responsibilities |
|
|
82 | (1) |
|
3.6.4 Professional Ethics |
|
|
83 | (2) |
|
|
85 | (1) |
|
3.8 Questions for Discussion |
|
|
86 | (1) |
|
4 Contexts for Clinical Trials |
|
|
87 | (50) |
|
|
87 | (4) |
|
4.1.1 Clinical Trial Registries |
|
|
88 | (2) |
|
4.1.2 Public Perception Versus Science |
|
|
90 | (1) |
|
|
91 | (4) |
|
|
92 | (1) |
|
4.2.2 Why Trials Are Used Extensively for Drugs |
|
|
93 | (2) |
|
|
95 | (4) |
|
4.3.1 Use of Trials for Medical Devices |
|
|
95 | (2) |
|
4.3.2 Are Devices Different from Drugs? |
|
|
97 | (1) |
|
|
98 | (1) |
|
|
99 | (7) |
|
4.4.1 The Prevention versus Therapy Dichotomy Is Over-worked |
|
|
100 | (1) |
|
4.4.2 Vaccines and Biologicals |
|
|
101 | (1) |
|
4.4.3 Ebola 2014 and Beyond |
|
|
102 | (1) |
|
4.4.4 A Perspective on Risk-Benefit |
|
|
103 | (2) |
|
4.4.5 Methodology and Framework for Prevention Trials |
|
|
105 | (1) |
|
4.5 Complementary and Alternative Medicine |
|
|
106 | (10) |
|
4.5.1 Science Is the Study of Natural Phenomena |
|
|
108 | (1) |
|
4.5.2 Ignorance Is Important |
|
|
109 | (1) |
|
4.5.3 The Essential Paradox of CAM and Clinical Trials |
|
|
110 | (1) |
|
4.5.4 Why Trials Have Not Been Used Extensively in CAM |
|
|
111 | (2) |
|
4.5.5 Some Principles for Rigorous Evaluation |
|
|
113 | (2) |
|
|
115 | (1) |
|
4.6 Surgery and Skill-Dependent Therapies |
|
|
116 | (14) |
|
4.6.1 Why Trials Have Been Used Less Extensively in Surgery |
|
|
118 | (2) |
|
4.6.2 Reasons Why Some Surgical Therapies Require Less Rigorous Study Designs |
|
|
120 | (1) |
|
4.6.3 Sources of Variation |
|
|
121 | (1) |
|
4.6.4 Difficulties of Inference |
|
|
121 | (1) |
|
4.6.5 Control of Observer Bias Is Possible |
|
|
122 | (2) |
|
4.6.6 Illustrations from an Emphysema Surgery Trial |
|
|
124 | (6) |
|
4.7 A Brief View of Some Other Contexts |
|
|
130 | (5) |
|
|
130 | (4) |
|
|
134 | (1) |
|
|
134 | (1) |
|
|
135 | (1) |
|
4.9 Questions for Discussion |
|
|
136 | (1) |
|
|
137 | (35) |
|
|
137 | (3) |
|
5.1.1 Types of Uncertainty |
|
|
138 | (2) |
|
|
140 | (3) |
|
5.2.1 Estimation Is The Most Common Objective |
|
|
141 | (1) |
|
5.2.2 Selection Can Also Be an Objective |
|
|
141 | (1) |
|
5.2.3 Objectives Require Various Scales of Measurement |
|
|
142 | (1) |
|
|
143 | (19) |
|
5.3.1 Mixed Outcomes and Predictors |
|
|
143 | (1) |
|
5.3.2 Criteria for Evaluating Outcomes |
|
|
144 | (1) |
|
5.3.3 Prefer Hard or Objective Outcomes |
|
|
145 | (1) |
|
5.3.4 Outcomes Can Be Quantitative or Qualitative |
|
|
146 | (1) |
|
5.3.5 Measures Are Useful and Efficient Outcomes |
|
|
146 | (1) |
|
5.3.6 Some Outcomes Are Summarized as Counts |
|
|
147 | (1) |
|
5.3.7 Ordered Categories Are Commonly Used for Severity or Toxicity |
|
|
147 | (1) |
|
5.3.8 Unordered Categories Are Sometimes Used |
|
|
148 | (1) |
|
5.3.9 Dichotomies Are Simple Summaries |
|
|
148 | (1) |
|
|
149 | (4) |
|
5.3.11 Primary and Others |
|
|
153 | (1) |
|
|
154 | (1) |
|
5.3.13 Event Times and Censoring |
|
|
155 | (5) |
|
5.3.14 Longitudinal Measures |
|
|
160 | (1) |
|
|
161 | (1) |
|
5.3.16 Patient Reported Outcomes |
|
|
161 | (1) |
|
|
162 | (8) |
|
5.4.1 Surrogate Outcomes Are Disease-Specific |
|
|
164 | (3) |
|
5.4.2 Surrogate Outcomes Can Make Trials More Efficient |
|
|
167 | (1) |
|
5.4.3 Surrogate Outcomes Have Significant Limitations |
|
|
168 | (2) |
|
|
170 | (1) |
|
5.6 Questions for Discussion |
|
|
171 | (1) |
|
|
172 | (24) |
|
|
172 | (9) |
|
6.1.1 The Effects of Random and Systematic Errors Are Distinct |
|
|
173 | (1) |
|
6.1.2 Hypothesis Tests versus Significance Tests |
|
|
174 | (1) |
|
6.1.3 Hypothesis Tests Are Subject to Two Types of Random Error |
|
|
175 | (1) |
|
6.1.4 Type I Errors Are Relatively Easy to Control |
|
|
176 | (1) |
|
6.1.5 The Properties of Confidence Intervals Are Similar to Hypothesis Tests |
|
|
176 | (1) |
|
6.1.6 Using a one- or two-sided hypothesis test is not the right question |
|
|
177 | (1) |
|
6.1.7 P-Values Quantify the Type I Error |
|
|
178 | (1) |
|
6.1.8 Type II Errors Depend on the Clinical Difference of Interest |
|
|
178 | (2) |
|
6.1.9 Post Hoc Power Calculations Are Useless |
|
|
180 | (1) |
|
|
181 | (7) |
|
6.2.1 Relative Size of Random Error and Bias is Important |
|
|
182 | (1) |
|
6.2.2 Bias Arises from Numerous Sources |
|
|
182 | (3) |
|
6.2.3 Controlling Structural Bias is Conceptually Simple |
|
|
185 | (3) |
|
|
188 | (6) |
|
|
188 | (4) |
|
6.3.2 Some Statistical Bias Can Be Corrected |
|
|
192 | (1) |
|
6.3.3 Unbiasedness is Not the Only Desirable Attribute of an Estimator |
|
|
192 | (2) |
|
|
194 | (1) |
|
6.5 Questions for Discussion |
|
|
194 | (2) |
|
7 Statistical Perspectives |
|
|
196 | (21) |
|
|
196 | (1) |
|
7.2 Differences in Statistical Perspectives |
|
|
197 | (5) |
|
7.2.1 Models and Parameters |
|
|
197 | (1) |
|
7.2.2 Philosophy of Inference Divides Statisticians |
|
|
198 | (1) |
|
|
199 | (1) |
|
7.2.4 Points of Agreement |
|
|
199 | (3) |
|
|
202 | (2) |
|
7.3.1 Binomial Case Study |
|
|
203 | (1) |
|
|
204 | (1) |
|
|
204 | (6) |
|
7.4.1 Choice of a Prior Distribution Is a Source of Contention |
|
|
205 | (1) |
|
7.4.2 Binomial Case Study |
|
|
206 | (3) |
|
7.4.3 Bayesian Inference Is Different |
|
|
209 | (1) |
|
|
210 | (2) |
|
7.5.1 Binomial Case Study |
|
|
211 | (1) |
|
7.5.2 Likelihood-Based Design |
|
|
211 | (1) |
|
|
212 | (3) |
|
|
212 | (1) |
|
7.6.2 Statistical Procedures Are Not Standardized |
|
|
213 | (1) |
|
7.6.3 Practical Controversies Related to Statistics Exist |
|
|
214 | (1) |
|
|
215 | (1) |
|
7.8 Questions for Discussion |
|
|
216 | (1) |
|
8 Experiment Design in Clinical Trials |
|
|
217 | (37) |
|
|
217 | (1) |
|
8.2 Trials As Simple Experiment Designs |
|
|
218 | (5) |
|
8.2.1 Design Space Is Chaotic |
|
|
219 | (1) |
|
8.2.2 Design Is Critical for Inference |
|
|
220 | (1) |
|
8.2.3 The Question Drives the Design |
|
|
220 | (1) |
|
8.2.4 Design Depends on the Observation Model As Well As the Biological Question |
|
|
221 | (1) |
|
|
222 | (1) |
|
8.3 Goals of Experiment Design |
|
|
223 | (2) |
|
8.3.1 Control of Random Error and Bias Is the Goal |
|
|
223 | (1) |
|
8.3.2 Conceptual Simplicity Is Also a Goal |
|
|
223 | (1) |
|
8.3.3 Encapsulation of Subjectivity |
|
|
224 | (1) |
|
|
225 | (1) |
|
|
225 | (5) |
|
8.4.1 The Foundations of Design Are Observation and Theory |
|
|
226 | (1) |
|
8.4.2 A Lesson from the Women's Health Initiative |
|
|
227 | (2) |
|
8.4.3 Experiments Use Three Components of Design |
|
|
229 | (1) |
|
|
230 | (7) |
|
|
231 | (1) |
|
|
232 | (1) |
|
8.5.3 Experimental and Observational Units |
|
|
232 | (1) |
|
8.5.4 Treatments and Factors |
|
|
233 | (1) |
|
|
233 | (1) |
|
|
234 | (1) |
|
|
234 | (1) |
|
|
235 | (1) |
|
|
236 | (1) |
|
8.6 Special Design Issues |
|
|
237 | (7) |
|
|
237 | (3) |
|
8.6.2 Equivalence and Noninferiority |
|
|
240 | (1) |
|
8.6.3 Randomized Discontinuation |
|
|
241 | (1) |
|
8.6.4 Hybrid Designs May Be Needed for Resolving Special Questions |
|
|
242 | (1) |
|
8.6.5 Clinical Trials Cannot Meet Certain Objectives |
|
|
242 | (2) |
|
8.7 Importance of the Protocol Document |
|
|
244 | (8) |
|
8.7.1 Protocols Have Many Functions |
|
|
244 | (1) |
|
8.7.2 Deviations from Protocol Specifications are Common |
|
|
245 | (1) |
|
8.7.3 Protocols Are Structured, Logical, and Complete |
|
|
246 | (6) |
|
|
252 | (1) |
|
8.9 Questions for Discussion |
|
|
253 | (1) |
|
|
254 | (23) |
|
|
254 | (1) |
|
9.2 Cohort Definition and Selection |
|
|
255 | (9) |
|
9.2.1 Eligibility and Exclusions |
|
|
255 | (2) |
|
9.2.2 Active Sampling and Enrichment |
|
|
257 | (1) |
|
9.2.3 Participation may select subjects with better prognosis |
|
|
258 | (4) |
|
9.2.4 Quantitative Selection Criteria Versus False Precision |
|
|
262 | (1) |
|
9.2.5 Comparative Trials Are Not Sensitive to Selection |
|
|
263 | (1) |
|
|
264 | (3) |
|
9.3.1 Using a Run-In Period |
|
|
264 | (1) |
|
9.3.2 Estimate Accrual Quantitatively |
|
|
265 | (2) |
|
9.4 Inclusiveness, Representation, and Interactions |
|
|
267 | (8) |
|
9.4.1 Inclusiveness Is a Worthy Goal |
|
|
267 | (1) |
|
9.4.2 Barriers Can Hinder Trial Participation |
|
|
268 | (1) |
|
9.4.3 Efficacy versus Effectiveness Trials |
|
|
269 | (1) |
|
9.4.4 Representation: Politics Blunders into Science |
|
|
270 | (5) |
|
|
275 | (1) |
|
9.6 Questions for Discussion |
|
|
275 | (2) |
|
|
277 | (25) |
|
|
277 | (4) |
|
10.1.1 Stages of Development |
|
|
278 | (2) |
|
10.1.2 Trial Design versus Development Design |
|
|
280 | (1) |
|
10.1.3 Companion Diagnostics in Cancer |
|
|
281 | (1) |
|
10.2 Pipeline Principles and Problems |
|
|
281 | (5) |
|
10.2.1 The Paradigm Is Not Linear |
|
|
282 | (1) |
|
10.2.2 Staging Allows Efficiency |
|
|
282 | (1) |
|
10.2.3 The Pipeline Impacts Study Design |
|
|
283 | (1) |
|
10.2.4 Specificity and Pressures Shape the Pipeline |
|
|
283 | (1) |
|
10.2.5 Problems with Trials |
|
|
284 | (2) |
|
10.2.6 Problems in the Pipeline |
|
|
286 | (1) |
|
10.3 A Simple Quantitative Pipeline |
|
|
286 | (6) |
|
10.3.1 Pipeline Operating Characteristics Can Be Derived |
|
|
286 | (2) |
|
10.3.2 Implications May Be Counterintuitive |
|
|
288 | (1) |
|
10.3.3 Optimization Yields Insights |
|
|
288 | (3) |
|
10.3.4 Overall Implications for the Pipeline |
|
|
291 | (1) |
|
|
292 | (8) |
|
10.4.1 Generic Mistakes in Evaluating Evidence |
|
|
293 | (1) |
|
10.4.2 "Safety" Begets Efficacy Testing |
|
|
293 | (1) |
|
10.4.3 Pressure to Advance Ideas Is Unprecedented |
|
|
294 | (1) |
|
10.4.4 Scientists Believe Weird Things |
|
|
294 | (1) |
|
|
295 | (1) |
|
10.4.6 Many Biological Endpoints Are Neither Predictive nor Prognostic |
|
|
296 | (1) |
|
10.4.7 Disbelief Is Easier to Suspend Than Belief |
|
|
296 | (1) |
|
|
297 | (1) |
|
10.4.9 Intellectual Conflicts of Interest |
|
|
297 | (1) |
|
10.4.10 Many Preclinical Models Are Invalid |
|
|
298 | (1) |
|
10.4.11 Variation Despite Genomic Determinism |
|
|
299 | (1) |
|
10.4.12 Weak Evidence Is Likely to Mislead |
|
|
300 | (1) |
|
|
300 | (1) |
|
10.6 Questions for Discussion |
|
|
301 | (1) |
|
11 Translational Clinical Trials |
|
|
302 | (27) |
|
|
302 | (6) |
|
11.1.1 Therapeutic Intent or Not? |
|
|
303 | (1) |
|
11.1.2 Mechanistic Trials |
|
|
304 | (1) |
|
11.1.3 Marker Threshold Designs Are Strongly Biased |
|
|
305 | (3) |
|
11.2 Inferential Paradigms |
|
|
308 | (4) |
|
|
308 | (2) |
|
|
310 | (1) |
|
11.2.3 Surrogate Paradigm |
|
|
311 | (1) |
|
|
312 | (1) |
|
11.3.1 Biological Models Are a Key to Translational Trials |
|
|
313 | (1) |
|
11.4 Translational Trials Defined |
|
|
313 | (4) |
|
11.4.1 Translational Paradigm |
|
|
313 | (2) |
|
11.4.2 Character and Definition |
|
|
315 | (1) |
|
11.4.3 Small or "Pilot" Does Not Mean Translational |
|
|
316 | (1) |
|
11.4.4 Hypothetical Example |
|
|
316 | (1) |
|
11.4.5 Nesting Translational Studies |
|
|
317 | (1) |
|
11.5 Information From Translational Trials |
|
|
317 | (11) |
|
11.5.1 Surprise Can Be Defined Mathematically |
|
|
318 | (1) |
|
11.5.2 Parameter Uncertainty Versus Outcome Uncertainty |
|
|
318 | (1) |
|
11.5.3 Expected Surprise and Entropy |
|
|
319 | (2) |
|
11.5.4 Information/Entropy Calculated From Small Samples Is Biased |
|
|
321 | (1) |
|
11.5.5 Variance of Information/Entropy |
|
|
322 | (2) |
|
11.5.6 Sample Size for Translational Trials |
|
|
324 | (3) |
|
|
327 | (1) |
|
|
328 | (1) |
|
11.7 Questions for Discussion |
|
|
328 | (1) |
|
12 Early Development and Dose-Finding |
|
|
329 | (41) |
|
|
329 | (1) |
|
|
330 | (3) |
|
12.2.1 Therapeutic Intent |
|
|
330 | (1) |
|
|
331 | (1) |
|
12.2.3 Dose versus Efficacy |
|
|
332 | (1) |
|
12.3 Essential Concepts for Dose versus Risk |
|
|
333 | (5) |
|
12.3.1 What Does the Terminology Mean? |
|
|
333 | (1) |
|
12.3.2 Distinguish Dose-Risk From Dose-Efficacy |
|
|
334 | (1) |
|
12.3.3 Dose Optimality Is a Design Definition |
|
|
335 | (1) |
|
12.3.4 Unavoidable Subjectivity |
|
|
335 | (1) |
|
12.3.5 Sample Size Is an Outcome of Dose-Finding Studies |
|
|
336 | (1) |
|
12.3.6 Idealized Dose-Finding Design |
|
|
336 | (2) |
|
|
338 | (6) |
|
12.4.1 Some Historical Designs |
|
|
338 | (1) |
|
12.4.2 Typical Dose-Ranging Design |
|
|
339 | (1) |
|
12.4.3 Operating Characteristics Can Be Calculated |
|
|
340 | (3) |
|
12.4.4 Modifications, Strengths, and Weaknesses |
|
|
343 | (1) |
|
12.5 Dose-Finding Is Model Based |
|
|
344 | (10) |
|
12.5.1 Mathematical Models Facilitate Inferences |
|
|
345 | (1) |
|
12.5.2 Continual Reassessment Method |
|
|
345 | (4) |
|
12.5.3 Pharmacokinetic Measurements Might Be Used to Improve CRM Dose Escalations |
|
|
349 | (1) |
|
12.5.4 The CRM Is an Attractive Design to Criticize |
|
|
350 | (1) |
|
12.5.5 CRM Clinical Examples |
|
|
350 | (1) |
|
12.5.6 Dose Distributions |
|
|
351 | (1) |
|
12.5.7 Estimation with Overdose Control (EWOC) |
|
|
351 | (2) |
|
12.5.8 Randomization in Early Development? |
|
|
353 | (1) |
|
12.5.9 Phase I Data Have Other Uses |
|
|
353 | (1) |
|
12.6 General Dose-Finding Issues |
|
|
354 | (12) |
|
12.6.1 The General Dose-Finding Problem Is Unsolved |
|
|
354 | (2) |
|
12.6.2 More than One Drug |
|
|
356 | (5) |
|
12.6.3 More than One Outcome |
|
|
361 | (2) |
|
12.6.4 Envelope Simulation |
|
|
363 | (3) |
|
|
366 | (2) |
|
12.8 Questions for Discussion |
|
|
368 | (2) |
|
|
370 | (27) |
|
|
370 | (2) |
|
13.1.1 Estimate Treatment Effects |
|
|
371 | (1) |
|
13.2 Characteristics of Middle Development |
|
|
372 | (3) |
|
|
373 | (1) |
|
|
374 | (1) |
|
|
375 | (1) |
|
|
375 | (4) |
|
13.3.1 Choices in Middle Development |
|
|
375 | (1) |
|
13.3.2 When to Skip Middle Development |
|
|
376 | (1) |
|
|
377 | (1) |
|
13.3.4 Other Design Issues |
|
|
378 | (1) |
|
13.4 Middle Development Distills True Positives |
|
|
379 | (2) |
|
13.5 Futility and Nonsuperiority Designs |
|
|
381 | (4) |
|
13.5.1 Asymmetry in Error Control |
|
|
382 | (1) |
|
13.5.2 Should We Control False Positives or False Negatives? |
|
|
383 | (1) |
|
13.5.3 Futility Design Example |
|
|
384 | (1) |
|
13.5.4 A Conventional Approach to Futility |
|
|
385 | (1) |
|
13.6 Dose-Efficacy Questions |
|
|
385 | (1) |
|
13.7 Randomized Comparisons |
|
|
386 | (6) |
|
13.7.1 When to Perform an Error-Prone Comparative Trial |
|
|
387 | (1) |
|
|
388 | (1) |
|
13.7.3 Randomized Selection |
|
|
389 | (3) |
|
|
392 | (3) |
|
|
395 | (1) |
|
13.10 Questions for Discussion |
|
|
396 | (1) |
|
|
397 | (16) |
|
|
397 | (1) |
|
14.2 Elements of Reliability |
|
|
398 | (4) |
|
|
399 | (1) |
|
|
400 | (1) |
|
14.2.3 Other Design Issues |
|
|
400 | (2) |
|
14.3 Biomarker-Based Comparative Designs |
|
|
402 | (6) |
|
14.3.1 Biomarkers Are Diverse |
|
|
402 | (2) |
|
|
404 | (1) |
|
14.3.3 Biomarker-Stratified |
|
|
404 | (1) |
|
14.3.4 Biomarker-Strategy |
|
|
405 | (1) |
|
14.3.5 Multiple-Biomarker Signal-Finding |
|
|
406 | (1) |
|
14.3.6 Prospective-Retrospective Evaluation of a Biomarker |
|
|
407 | (1) |
|
|
407 | (1) |
|
14.4 Some Special Comparative Designs |
|
|
408 | (3) |
|
14.4.1 Randomized Discontinuation |
|
|
408 | (1) |
|
|
409 | (1) |
|
14.4.3 Cluster Randomization |
|
|
410 | (1) |
|
|
410 | (1) |
|
14.4.5 Multiple Agents versus Control |
|
|
410 | (1) |
|
|
411 | (1) |
|
14.6 Questions for Discussion |
|
|
412 | (1) |
|
15 Adaptive Design Features |
|
|
413 | (17) |
|
|
413 | (5) |
|
15.1.1 Advantages and Disadvantages of AD |
|
|
414 | (2) |
|
15.1.2 Design Adaptations Are Tools, Not a Class |
|
|
416 | (1) |
|
15.1.3 Perspective on Bayesian Methods |
|
|
417 | (1) |
|
15.1.4 The Pipeline Is the Main Adaptive Tool |
|
|
417 | (1) |
|
15.2 Some Familiar Adaptations |
|
|
418 | (5) |
|
15.2.1 Dose-Finding Is Adaptive |
|
|
418 | (1) |
|
15.2.2 Adaptive Randomization |
|
|
418 | (4) |
|
15.2.3 Staging is Adaptive |
|
|
422 | (1) |
|
15.2.4 Dropping a Treatment Arm or Subset |
|
|
423 | (1) |
|
15.3 Biomarker Adaptive Trials |
|
|
423 | (2) |
|
|
425 | (2) |
|
15.4.1 Sample Size Re-Estimation Requires Caution |
|
|
425 | (2) |
|
|
427 | (1) |
|
15.6 Barriers to the Use of AD |
|
|
428 | (1) |
|
15.7 Adaptive Design Case Study |
|
|
428 | (1) |
|
|
429 | (1) |
|
15.9 Questions for Discussion |
|
|
429 | (1) |
|
|
430 | (62) |
|
|
430 | (1) |
|
|
431 | (5) |
|
16.2.1 What Is Precision? |
|
|
432 | (1) |
|
|
433 | (1) |
|
|
434 | (1) |
|
16.2.4 Sample Size and Power Calculations Are Approximations |
|
|
435 | (1) |
|
16.2.5 The Relationship between Power/Precision and Sample Size Is Quadratic |
|
|
435 | (1) |
|
16.3 Early Developmental Trials |
|
|
436 | (2) |
|
16.3.1 Translational Trials |
|
|
436 | (1) |
|
16.3.2 Dose-Finding Trials |
|
|
437 | (1) |
|
16.4 Simple Estimation Designs |
|
|
438 | (13) |
|
16.4.1 Confidence Intervals for a Mean Provide a Sample Size Approach |
|
|
438 | (2) |
|
16.4.2 Estimating Proportions Accurately |
|
|
440 | (1) |
|
16.4.3 Exact Binomial Confidence Limits Are Helpful |
|
|
441 | (3) |
|
16.4.4 Precision Helps Detect Improvement |
|
|
444 | (2) |
|
16.4.5 Bayesian Binomial Confidence Intervals |
|
|
446 | (1) |
|
16.4.6 A Bayesian Approach Can Use Prior Information |
|
|
447 | (3) |
|
16.4.7 Likelihood-Based Approach for Proportions |
|
|
450 | (1) |
|
|
451 | (4) |
|
16.5.1 Confidence Intervals for Event Rates Can Determine Sample Size |
|
|
451 | (3) |
|
16.5.2 Likelihood-Based Approach for Event Rates |
|
|
454 | (1) |
|
|
455 | (2) |
|
16.6.1 Ineffective or Unsafe Treatments Should Be Discarded Early |
|
|
455 | (1) |
|
16.6.2 Two-Stage Designs Increase Efficiency |
|
|
456 | (1) |
|
|
457 | (21) |
|
16.7.1 How to Choose Type I and II Error Rates? |
|
|
459 | (1) |
|
16.7.2 Comparisons Using the r-Test Are a Good Learning Example |
|
|
459 | (3) |
|
16.7.3 Likelihood-Based Approach |
|
|
462 | (1) |
|
16.7.4 Dichotomous Responses Are More Complex |
|
|
463 | (1) |
|
16.7.5 Hazard Comparisons Yield Similar Equations |
|
|
464 | (3) |
|
16.7.6 Parametric and Nonparametric Equations Are Connected |
|
|
467 | (1) |
|
16.7.7 Accommodating Unbalanced Treatment Assignments |
|
|
467 | (2) |
|
16.7.8 A Simple Accrual Model Can Also Be Incorporated |
|
|
469 | (2) |
|
|
471 | (1) |
|
|
472 | (6) |
|
16.8 Expanded Safety Trials |
|
|
478 | (3) |
|
16.8.1 Model Rare Events with the Poisson Distribution |
|
|
479 | (1) |
|
16.8.2 Likelihood Approach for Poisson Rates |
|
|
479 | (2) |
|
16.9 Other Considerations |
|
|
481 | (8) |
|
16.9.1 Cluster Randomization Requires Increased Sample Size |
|
|
481 | (1) |
|
16.9.2 Simple Cost Optimization |
|
|
482 | (1) |
|
16.9.3 Increase the Sample Size for Nonadherence |
|
|
482 | (3) |
|
16.9.4 Simulated Lifetables Can Be a Simple Design Tool |
|
|
485 | (1) |
|
16.9.5 Sample Size for Prognostic Factor Studies |
|
|
486 | (1) |
|
16.9.6 Computer Programs Simplify Calculations |
|
|
487 | (1) |
|
16.9.7 Simulation Is a Powerful and Flexible Design Alternative |
|
|
487 | (1) |
|
16.9.8 Power Curves Are Sigmoid Shaped |
|
|
488 | (1) |
|
|
489 | (1) |
|
16.11 Questions for Discussion |
|
|
490 | (2) |
|
|
492 | (30) |
|
|
492 | (2) |
|
17.1.1 Balance and Bias Are Independent |
|
|
493 | (1) |
|
|
494 | (6) |
|
17.2.1 Heuristic Proof of the Value of Randomization |
|
|
495 | (2) |
|
17.2.2 Control the Influence of Unknown Factors |
|
|
497 | (1) |
|
17.2.3 Haphazard Assignments Are Not Random |
|
|
498 | (1) |
|
17.2.4 Simple Randomization Can Yield Imbalances |
|
|
499 | (1) |
|
17.3 Constrained Randomization |
|
|
500 | (4) |
|
17.3.1 Blocking Improves Balance |
|
|
500 | (1) |
|
17.3.2 Blocking and Stratifying Balances Prognostic Factors |
|
|
501 | (2) |
|
17.3.3 Other Considerations Regarding Blocking |
|
|
503 | (1) |
|
|
504 | (3) |
|
17.4.1 Urn Designs Also Improve Balance |
|
|
504 | (1) |
|
17.4.2 Minimization Yields Tight Balance |
|
|
504 | (1) |
|
|
505 | (2) |
|
17.5 Other Issues Regarding Randomization |
|
|
507 | (7) |
|
17.5.1 Administration of the Randomization |
|
|
507 | (1) |
|
17.5.2 Computers Generate Pseudorandom Numbers |
|
|
508 | (1) |
|
17.5.3 Randomized Treatment Assignment Justifies Type I Errors |
|
|
509 | (5) |
|
17.6 Unequal Treatment Allocation |
|
|
514 | (5) |
|
17.6.1 Subsets May Be of Interest |
|
|
514 | (1) |
|
17.6.2 Treatments May Differ Greatly in Cost |
|
|
515 | (1) |
|
17.6.3 Variances May Be Different |
|
|
515 | (1) |
|
17.6.4 Multiarm Trials May Require Asymmetric Allocation |
|
|
516 | (1) |
|
|
517 | (1) |
|
17.6.6 Failed Randomization? |
|
|
518 | (1) |
|
17.7 Randomization Before Consent |
|
|
519 | (1) |
|
|
520 | (1) |
|
17.9 Questions for Discussion |
|
|
520 | (2) |
|
18 Treatment Effects Monitoring |
|
|
522 | (51) |
|
|
522 | (5) |
|
18.1.1 Motives for Monitoring |
|
|
523 | (1) |
|
18.1.2 Components of Responsible Monitoring |
|
|
524 | (1) |
|
18.1.3 Trials Can Be Stopped for a Variety of Reasons |
|
|
524 | (2) |
|
18.1.4 There Is Tension in the Decision to Stop |
|
|
526 | (1) |
|
18.2 Administrative Issues in Trial Monitoring |
|
|
527 | (10) |
|
18.2.1 Monitoring of Single-Center Studies Relies on Periodic Investigator Reporting |
|
|
527 | (1) |
|
18.2.2 Composition and Organization of the TEMC |
|
|
528 | (7) |
|
18.2.3 Complete Objectivity Is Not Ethical |
|
|
535 | (2) |
|
18.2.4 Independent Experts in Monitoring |
|
|
537 | (1) |
|
18.3 Organizational Issues Related to Monitoring |
|
|
537 | (8) |
|
18.3.1 Initial TEMC Meeting |
|
|
538 | (1) |
|
18.3.2 The TEMC Assesses Baseline Comparability |
|
|
538 | (1) |
|
18.3.3 The TEMC Reviews Accrual and Expected Time to Study Completion |
|
|
539 | (1) |
|
18.3.4 Timeliness of Data and Reporting Lags |
|
|
539 | (1) |
|
18.3.5 Data Quality Is a Major Focus of the TEMC |
|
|
540 | (1) |
|
18.3.6 The TEMC Reviews Safety and Toxicity Data |
|
|
541 | (1) |
|
18.3.7 Efficacy Differences Are Assessed by the TEMC |
|
|
541 | (1) |
|
18.3.8 The TEMC Should Address Some Practical Questions Specifically |
|
|
541 | (3) |
|
18.3.9 The TEMC Mechanism Has Potential Weaknesses |
|
|
544 | (1) |
|
18.4 Statistical Methods for Monitoring |
|
|
545 | (25) |
|
18.4.1 There Are Several Approaches to Evaluating Incomplete Evidence |
|
|
545 | (2) |
|
18.4.2 Monitoring Developmental Trials for Risk |
|
|
547 | (4) |
|
18.4.3 Likelihood-Based Methods |
|
|
551 | (6) |
|
|
557 | (2) |
|
18.4.5 Decision-Theoretic Methods |
|
|
559 | (1) |
|
18.4.6 Frequentist Methods |
|
|
560 | (6) |
|
18.4.7 Other Monitoring Tools |
|
|
566 | (4) |
|
|
570 | (1) |
|
|
570 | (2) |
|
18.6 Questions for Discussion |
|
|
572 | (1) |
|
19 Counting Subjects and Events |
|
|
573 | (17) |
|
|
573 | (1) |
|
19.2 Imperfection and Validity |
|
|
574 | (1) |
|
19.3 Treatment Nonadherence |
|
|
575 | (5) |
|
19.3.1 Intention to Treat Is a Policy of Inclusion |
|
|
575 | (1) |
|
19.3.2 Coronary Drug Project Results Illustrate the Pitfalls of Exclusions Based on Nonadherence |
|
|
576 | (1) |
|
19.3.3 Statistical Studies Support the ITT Approach |
|
|
577 | (1) |
|
19.3.4 Trials Are Tests of Treatment Policy |
|
|
577 | (1) |
|
19.3.5 ITT Analyses Cannot Always Be Applied |
|
|
578 | (1) |
|
19.3.6 Trial Inferences Depend on the Experiment Design |
|
|
579 | (1) |
|
19.4 Protocol Nonadherence |
|
|
580 | (3) |
|
|
580 | (1) |
|
|
581 | (1) |
|
19.4.3 Defects in Retrospect |
|
|
582 | (1) |
|
|
583 | (5) |
|
19.5.1 Evaluability Criteria Are a Methodologic Error |
|
|
583 | (1) |
|
19.5.2 Statistical Methods Can Cope with Some Types of Missing Data |
|
|
584 | (4) |
|
|
588 | (1) |
|
19.7 Questions for Discussion |
|
|
589 | (1) |
|
20 Estimating Clinical Effects |
|
|
590 | (54) |
|
|
590 | (4) |
|
20.1.1 Invisibility Works Against Validity |
|
|
591 | (1) |
|
20.1.2 Structure Aids Internal and External Validity |
|
|
591 | (1) |
|
20.1.3 Estimates of Risk Are Natural and Useful |
|
|
592 | (2) |
|
20.2 Dose-Finding and Pharmacokinetic Trials |
|
|
594 | (5) |
|
20.2.1 Pharmacokinetic Models Are Essential for Analyzing DF Trials |
|
|
594 | (1) |
|
20.2.2 A Two-Compartment Model Is Simple but Realistic |
|
|
595 | (3) |
|
20.2.3 PK Models Are Used By "Model Fitting" |
|
|
598 | (1) |
|
20.3 Middle Development Studies |
|
|
599 | (7) |
|
20.3.1 Mesothelioma Clinical Trial Example |
|
|
599 | (1) |
|
20.3.2 Summarize Risk for Dichotomous Factors |
|
|
600 | (1) |
|
20.3.3 Nonparametric Estimates of Survival Are Robust |
|
|
601 | (2) |
|
20.3.4 Parametric (Exponential) Summaries of Survival Are Efficient |
|
|
603 | (2) |
|
20.3.5 Percent Change and Waterfall Plots |
|
|
605 | (1) |
|
20.4 Randomized Comparative Trials |
|
|
606 | (10) |
|
20.4.1 Examples of Comparative Trials Used in This Section |
|
|
607 | (1) |
|
20.4.2 Continuous Measures Estimate Treatment Differences |
|
|
608 | (1) |
|
20.4.3 Baseline Measurements Can Increase Precision |
|
|
609 | (1) |
|
|
610 | (2) |
|
20.4.5 Nonparametric Survival Comparisons |
|
|
612 | (2) |
|
20.4.6 Risk (Hazard) Ratios and Confidence Intervals Are Clinically Useful Data Summaries |
|
|
614 | (1) |
|
20.4.7 Statistical Models Are Necessary Tools |
|
|
615 | (1) |
|
20.5 Problems With P-Values |
|
|
616 | (4) |
|
20.5.1 P-Values Do Not Represent Treatment Effects |
|
|
618 | (1) |
|
20.5.2 P-Values Do Not Imply Reproducibility |
|
|
618 | (1) |
|
20.5.3 P-Values Do Not Measure Evidence |
|
|
619 | (1) |
|
20.6 Strength of Evidence Through Support Intervals |
|
|
620 | (2) |
|
20.6.1 Support Intervals Are Based on the Likelihood Function |
|
|
620 | (1) |
|
20.6.2 Support Intervals Can Be Used with Any Outcome |
|
|
621 | (1) |
|
20.7 Special Methods of Analysis |
|
|
622 | (6) |
|
20.7.1 The Bootstrap Is Based on Resampling |
|
|
623 | (1) |
|
20.7.2 Some Clinical Questions Require Other Special Methods of Analysis |
|
|
623 | (5) |
|
20.8 Exploratory Analyses |
|
|
628 | (11) |
|
20.8.1 Clinical Trial Data Lend Themselves to Exploratory Analyses |
|
|
628 | (1) |
|
20.8.2 Multiple Tests Multiply Type I Errors |
|
|
629 | (1) |
|
20.8.3 Kinds of Multiplicity |
|
|
630 | (1) |
|
20.8.4 Inevitible Risks from Subgroups |
|
|
630 | (2) |
|
20.8.5 Tale of a Subset Analysis Gone Wrong |
|
|
632 | (3) |
|
20.8.6 Perspective on Subgroup Analyses |
|
|
635 | (1) |
|
20.8.7 Effects the Trial Was Not Designed to Detect |
|
|
636 | (1) |
|
|
637 | (1) |
|
|
637 | (1) |
|
|
638 | (1) |
|
|
639 | (1) |
|
20.10 Questions for Discussion |
|
|
640 | (4) |
|
21 Prognostic Factor Analyses |
|
|
644 | (27) |
|
|
644 | (3) |
|
21.1.1 Studying Prognostic Factors is Broadly Useful |
|
|
645 | (1) |
|
21.1.2 Prognostic Factors Can Be Constant or Time-Varying |
|
|
646 | (1) |
|
|
647 | (14) |
|
21.2.1 Models Combine Theory and Data |
|
|
647 | (1) |
|
21.2.2 Scale and Coding May Be Important |
|
|
648 | (1) |
|
21.2.3 Use Flexible Covariate Models |
|
|
648 | (2) |
|
21.2.4 Building Parsimonious Models Is the Next Step |
|
|
650 | (5) |
|
21.2.5 Incompletely Specified Models May Yield Biased Estimates |
|
|
655 | (1) |
|
21.2.6 Study Second-Order Effects (Interactions) |
|
|
656 | (1) |
|
21.2.7 PFAs Can Help Describe Risk Groups |
|
|
656 | (4) |
|
21.2.8 Power and Sample Size for PFAs |
|
|
660 | (1) |
|
21.3 Adjusted Analyses of Comparative Trials |
|
|
661 | (5) |
|
21.3.1 What Should We Adjust For? |
|
|
662 | (1) |
|
|
663 | (1) |
|
21.3.3 Brain Tumor Case Study |
|
|
664 | (2) |
|
|
666 | (3) |
|
21.4.1 Recursive Partitioning Uses Dichotomies |
|
|
666 | (1) |
|
21.4.2 Neural Networks Are Used for Pattern Recognition |
|
|
667 | (2) |
|
|
669 | (1) |
|
21.6 Questions for Discussion |
|
|
669 | (2) |
|
|
671 | (13) |
|
|
671 | (1) |
|
22.2 Characteristics of Factorial Designs |
|
|
672 | (3) |
|
22.2.1 Interactions or Efficiency, But Not Both Simultaneously |
|
|
672 | (1) |
|
22.2.2 Factorial Designs Are Defined by Their Structure |
|
|
672 | (2) |
|
22.2.3 Factorial Designs Can Be Made Efficient |
|
|
674 | (1) |
|
22.3 Treatment Interactions |
|
|
675 | (5) |
|
22.3.1 Factorial Designs Are the Only Way to Study Interactions |
|
|
675 | (2) |
|
22.3.2 Interactions Depend on the Scale of Measurement |
|
|
677 | (1) |
|
22.3.3 The Interpretation of Main Effects Depends on Interactions |
|
|
677 | (1) |
|
22.3.4 Analyses Can Employ Linear Models |
|
|
678 | (2) |
|
22.4 Examples of Factorial Designs |
|
|
680 | (2) |
|
22.5 Partial, Fractional, and Incomplete Factorials |
|
|
682 | (1) |
|
22.5.1 Use Partial Factorial Designs When Interactions Are Absent |
|
|
682 | (1) |
|
22.5.2 Incomplete Designs Present Special Problems |
|
|
682 | (1) |
|
|
683 | (1) |
|
22.7 Questions for Discussion |
|
|
683 | (1) |
|
|
684 | (14) |
|
|
684 | (2) |
|
23.1.1 Other Ways of Giving Multiple Treatments Are Not Crossovers |
|
|
685 | (1) |
|
23.1.2 Treatment Periods May Be Randomly Assigned |
|
|
686 | (1) |
|
23.2 Advantages and Disadvantages |
|
|
686 | (5) |
|
23.2.1 Crossover Designs Can Increase Precision |
|
|
687 | (1) |
|
23.2.2 A Crossover Design Might Improve Recruitment |
|
|
687 | (1) |
|
23.2.3 Carryover Effects Are a Potential Problem |
|
|
688 | (1) |
|
23.2.4 Dropouts Have Strong Effects |
|
|
689 | (1) |
|
23.2.5 Analysis is More Complex Than for a Parallel-Group Design |
|
|
689 | (1) |
|
23.2.6 Prerequisites Are Needed to Apply Crossover Designs |
|
|
689 | (1) |
|
23.2.7 Other Uses for the Design |
|
|
690 | (1) |
|
|
691 | (5) |
|
|
691 | (1) |
|
23.3.2 Analysis Can Be Based on a Cell Means Model |
|
|
692 | (4) |
|
23.3.3 Other Issues in Analysis |
|
|
696 | (1) |
|
|
696 | (1) |
|
|
696 | (1) |
|
23.6 Questions for Discussion |
|
|
697 | (1) |
|
|
698 | (11) |
|
|
698 | (2) |
|
24.1.1 Meta-Analyses Formalize Synthesis and Increase Precision |
|
|
699 | (1) |
|
24.2 A Sketch of Meta-Analysis Methods |
|
|
700 | (5) |
|
24.2.1 Meta-Analysis Necessitates Prerequisites |
|
|
700 | (1) |
|
24.2.2 Many Studies Are Potentially Relevant |
|
|
701 | (1) |
|
|
702 | (1) |
|
24.2.4 Plan the Statistical Analysis |
|
|
703 | (1) |
|
24.2.5 Summarize the Data Using Observed and Expected |
|
|
703 | (2) |
|
|
705 | (2) |
|
24.3.1 Cumulative Meta-Analyses |
|
|
705 | (1) |
|
24.3.2 Meta-Analyses Have Practical and Theoretical Limitations |
|
|
706 | (1) |
|
24.3.3 Meta-Analysis Has Taught Useful Lessons |
|
|
707 | (1) |
|
|
707 | (1) |
|
24.5 Questions for Discussion |
|
|
708 | (1) |
|
25 Reporting and Authorship |
|
|
709 | (25) |
|
|
709 | (1) |
|
25.2 General Issues in Reporting |
|
|
710 | (5) |
|
25.2.1 Uniformity Improves Comprehension |
|
|
711 | (1) |
|
25.2.2 Quality of the Literature |
|
|
712 | (1) |
|
25.2.3 Peer Review Is the Only Game in Town |
|
|
712 | (1) |
|
25.2.4 Publication Bias Can Distort Impressions Based on the Literature |
|
|
713 | (2) |
|
25.3 Clinical Trial Reports |
|
|
715 | (11) |
|
25.3.1 General Considerations |
|
|
716 | (5) |
|
25.3.2 Employ a Complete Outline for Comparative Trial Reporting |
|
|
721 | (5) |
|
|
726 | (5) |
|
25.4.1 Inclusion and Ordering |
|
|
727 | (1) |
|
25.4.2 Responsibility of Authorship |
|
|
727 | (1) |
|
|
728 | (2) |
|
25.4.4 Some Other Practicalities |
|
|
730 | (1) |
|
25.5 Other Issues in Disseminating Results |
|
|
731 | (1) |
|
|
731 | (1) |
|
|
731 | (1) |
|
|
732 | (1) |
|
|
732 | (1) |
|
25.7 Questions for Discussion |
|
|
733 | (1) |
|
26 Misconduct and Fraud in Clinical Research |
|
|
734 | (27) |
|
|
734 | (7) |
|
26.1.1 Integrity and Accountability Are Critically Important |
|
|
736 | (2) |
|
26.1.2 Fraud and Misconduct Are Difficult to Define |
|
|
738 | (3) |
|
|
741 | (2) |
|
26.2.1 Misconduct May Be Increasing in Frequency |
|
|
741 | (1) |
|
26.2.2 Causes of Misconduct |
|
|
742 | (1) |
|
26.3 Approach to Allegations of Misconduct |
|
|
743 | (4) |
|
|
744 | (2) |
|
|
746 | (1) |
|
26.4 Characteristics of Some Misconduct Cases |
|
|
747 | (7) |
|
|
747 | (2) |
|
26.4.2 Poisson (NSABP) Case |
|
|
749 | (3) |
|
26.4.3 Two Recent Cases from Germany |
|
|
752 | (1) |
|
|
753 | (1) |
|
|
754 | (1) |
|
|
754 | (3) |
|
26.5.1 Recognizing Fraud or Misconduct |
|
|
754 | (2) |
|
26.5.2 Misconduct Cases Yield Other Lessons |
|
|
756 | (1) |
|
26.6 Clinical Investigators' Responsibilities |
|
|
757 | (2) |
|
26.6.1 General Responsibilities |
|
|
757 | (1) |
|
26.6.2 Additional Responsibilities Related to INDs |
|
|
758 | (1) |
|
26.6.3 Sponsor Responsibilities |
|
|
759 | (1) |
|
|
759 | (1) |
|
26.8 Questions for Discussion |
|
|
760 | (1) |
|
Appendix A Data and Programs |
|
|
761 | (3) |
|
|
761 | (1) |
|
|
761 | (2) |
|
A.2.1 Power and Sample Size Program |
|
|
761 | (2) |
|
A.2.2 Blocked Stratified Randomization |
|
|
763 | (1) |
|
A.2.3 Continual Reassessment Method |
|
|
763 | (1) |
|
A.2.4 Envelope Simulation |
|
|
763 | (1) |
|
|
763 | (1) |
|
|
764 | (5) |
|
Appendix C Notation and Terminology |
|
|
769 | (19) |
|
|
769 | (1) |
|
|
769 | (3) |
|
|
770 | (1) |
|
|
771 | (1) |
|
|
772 | (1) |
|
C.3 Terminology and Concepts |
|
|
772 | (16) |
|
Appendix D Nuremberg Code |
|
|
788 | (2) |
|
D. 1 Permissible Medical Experiments |
|
|
788 | (2) |
References |
|
790 | (81) |
Index |
|
871 | |